My research is supported by a UKRI Future Leaders Fellowship on Collective Behaviour of Cognitive Agents. The goal of this fellowship is to use Bayesian inference and decision-making, game theory and strategic artificial intelligence to develop models of social behaviour by rational agents.
|
2021
Count your peers or follow the person in front? What social information is most useful when making a decision? Should you weigh up how many people have chosen each option, or follow the most recent decision? In this paper, published in J R Soc Interface (Mann 2021) I show how a rational agent would use different types of social information to make the best decision it can. These range in complexity from a full sequence of previous decisions to simply watching the last decision made. Testing the different strategies against each other, I find that counting decisions is useful when that information is relatively cheap, but following the person in front makes sense when observing what others do is difficult.
|
Why you can stop worrying about which queue is moving faster. In this paper, published in PLoS Computational Biology (Mann 2021), I consider the problem of agents choosing where to forage, in competition with others. This classic problem can model animals selecting where to graze, but also people choosing which investments to buy, which route to take to work, or which queue to join. I show that if there is a cost associated with identifying which option is better (such as the time taken to select the fastest queue), some agents will adapt by choosing randomly and avoid paying that cost, relying on the competition among others to make the chocie largely irrelevant.
|
2020
Biases in molecular clocks and evidence for a genuine Cambrian Explosion. Fossil evidence of animal life appears suddenly in the late Ediacaran and rapidly diversifies in the early Cambrian, the so-called "Cambrian Explosion". This apparent rapid emergence of complex animal life has been questioned by phylogenetic-dating studies based on the DNA of modern animals, which have tended to place the origin of animals much deeper the past. In this paper in Interface Focus (Budd & Mann 2020) we show how biases caused by rapid early diversification can create a bias towards deeper origins in these 'molecular clock' studies. Furthermore, we show that the ordered appearance of fossil taxa is strong evidence for the veracity of the fossil record and against a cryptic early origin for the major animal groups.
|
When is it rational to follow others? Imagine standing in front of a large departures board at the train station. Suddenly everyone starts to move. Do you follow them? Or do you check the board, and realise that your train hasn't arrived yet? Now consider standing on the platform waiting for your train. You hear a muffled message on the intercom, and the other people on the platform who were waiting for the same train all start running. Do you follow? In this paper published in PNAS (Mann 2020) I show that we should follow others when information is poor (the muffled intercom) and when others want the same things we do (the same train). On the other hand, when we have good information (the departures board) and others might have different goals (be waiting for different trains), we should trust our own information and ignore what others are doing.
Photo: wikimedia commons |
Understanding the origins and extinction of major groups of plants and animals. How groups of organisms such as the animals and flowering plants come into being and diversify is a long standing question in evolutionary biology. In particular, the early origins of these groups presents a challenge: many seem to emerge rapidly in the fossil record, rather than gradually evolving over long periods of time. With respect to the flowering plants, Darwin even called this an 'abominable mystery', that challenged his gradualist idea of evolutionary change. This has prompted speculation that such organisms may have evolved slowly under special circumstances that prevented their appearance in the fossil record. In this paper, published in Science Advances (Budd & Mann 2020), we show that a rapid evolution is exactly what we should expect, that major groups establish themselves quickly and are only disturbed by later mass extinctions (such as that of the dinosaurs), giving rise to the characteristic flora and fauna of different geological periods.
|
2019

Virtual reality simulators predict clinical performance in dentistry. Prediction of how well a student will perform in clinical dental training is largely driven by measures of a student’s intellectual capabilities. The measurement of coordination, dexterity and precision of movement has lagged behind, despite being of obvious importance for core dental activities such as drill use. In this study in BMJ Simulation and Technology Enhanced Learning (Al-Saud et al. 2019) we used a virtual reality simulator called Simodont to measure student performance on simulated manual tasks, and showed that how well students did on these tasks was more strongly predictve of later clinical performance than traditional tests that are currently widely employed.

Understanding the rise of the far-right in Sweden. The Sweden Democrats (Sverigedemokraterna, SD) are the main far-right party in Sweden. In common with many far-right parties across Europe, they have seen an increase in support over the last decade, and have become a fixture in the Swedish parliament. While SD has increasingly tried to portray itself as a respectable political option, the increasing support for SD has produced great concern because of their roots in the fascist movement. Far-right support is often linked in the press to ethic competition, in particular concerns that immigrants are 'taking the jobs' from native citizens. In this paper published in Phil Trans Roy Soc A (Blomqvist, Sumpter & Mann 2019), we use demographic and electoral data from the Swedish Statistics Agency, along with a dynamical systems model, to identity what factors predict the rise of far-right support across Swedish municipalities. We find no basis for the ethnic competition hypothesis. Instead we find that education levels are the most important predictor of where SD's support has grown. This result mirrors analysis of the drivers of the Brexit vote in the UK, and suggests that mainstream responses to populism, such as reducing immigration, are misguided.

Inferring past predation rates from scars on fossil shells. Predation is an important driver of evolutionary adaptation. Prey species evolve defences such as shells, speed and camouflage, and predator species adapt by evolving new weapons against those defences. Since predation is so important in understanding how species have changed, we would like to be able to find out how much 'predation pressure' there was in the past. That is, we want to know how often prey animals were attacked, and how often those attacks were successful. A common way to estimate these parameters is to look at scarring on the fossils of prey animals, but previous work has lacked a full mathematical description of how scars accumulate. In this paper, published in Paleobiology (Budd & Mann 2019), we lay out a model of how these rates of attack and success lead to specific distributions of scars on the fossil shells of gastropods (snails), and we discuss many of the difficulties inherent in using these scar distributions to infer predation rates
2018

Rational decision-making generates complex collective behaviour. Have you ever read a tourist guide that suggests choosing a restaurant where the locals eat? Where does this cliché come from? We expect that if other people are choosing a restaurant they must know that it is better than others, especially if they know the area, so we assume that following them is a good idea. But we run the danger of all following each other into a mediocre restaurant just because that was the one the first person picked. In this paper, published in PNAS (Mann 2018), I model what a rational agent can learn from the decisions of others, when they should follow their peers or their own private information, and what the consequences are for groups of rational individuals. The resulting model throws up some surprising predictions about collective behaviour and interactions between agents that can be tested experimentally.

Identifying the factors behind neighbourhood segregation. Cities are often segregated into neighbourhoods composed of similar households. We are all familiar with the areas of our own city or town where the richest and poorest households live, and many cities have areas that are home to large immigrant populations or to distinctive populations of ethnic minorities, such as the many 'chinatowns' found around the world. In this paper, published in PLoS ONE (Mann et al. 2018), we looked at the factors that create and maintain this segregation. Do people tend to move to neighbourhoods populated with people similar to themselves? Using a new Gaussian process model to analyse 56,000 household moves in Stockholm, we find that in general the answer is yes, especially for native Swedes and for poorer ethnic minorities. This creates and maintains self-reinforcing segregation along ethnic and financial lines.

Citizen science reveals birds of prey match their hunting times to prey activity. The times when prey can safely forage or predators can successfully hunt depend on the presence or absence of the other. Do predators hunt when the prey are most active, or do they target prey when they are most vulnerable, such as after feeding? In this paper, published in Behavioral Ecology (Lang et al. 2018), we used records of bird sightings from bird watchers across the USA and Great Britain & Ireland, through the eBird and BirdTrack databases, to assess activity levels of different bird species, focusing on the connections between when prey species were active and the hunting strategies of their predators. We found that the pattern of times when prey birds were active were strongly predictive of when their specific predators were also seen, whereas other predators that mainly eat small mammals were active at other times.

The history of life was written by the victors. The diversity of life through time shows some striking patterns. For example, the animals appear in the fossil record about 550 million years ago, in an enormous burst of diversification called the “Cambrian Explosion”. Many groups of organisms appear to originate like this, but later on in their evolutionary history, their rates of diversification and morphological change seem to slow down. Our paper, published in Evolution (Budd & Mann 2018) argues that these patterns are a type of statistical artefact caused by our unavoidably recent viewpoint looking back into the past: only groups of animals that followed this pattern survived long enough to be represented in the fossil record. As a result, the patterns we discover by analysing such groups are not general features of evolution as a whole, but rather represent a remarkable bias that emerges by only studying groups we already know were successful. It follows that many traditional explanations for why diversity changes through time may need to be reconsidered. Photo by John Alan Elson CC BY-SA 4.0
Read more: Current Biology Dispatch
Watch: Graham Budd presents our work at PalAss
Read more: Current Biology Dispatch
Watch: Graham Budd presents our work at PalAss

Meat ants combine social and private information when foraging. Many species of ants use pheromone trails to communicate and help each other find food. When a foraging ant finds a good food source it will leave a trail of pheromone on its way back to the nest for others to follow. Ants can also remember where they have found food themselves before. In this study, published in Insectes Sociaux (Middleton et al. 2018) we conducted experiments that put private information (memory) in conflict with social information (pheromone). We found that when ants had one source of information alone they would follow it. However, if an ant had memory of food in one direction, but could detect pheromone in another, it would be equally likely to choose either direction, showing that the two sources of information had approximately equal weights.
Photo by CSIRO, CC BY 3.0
Read more: Insectes Sociaux editorial
Photo by CSIRO, CC BY 3.0
Read more: Insectes Sociaux editorial

Using neural networks to benchmark the predictions from models of international development. Understanding how nations develop both economically and socially is vitally important for predicting how the world will change in the 21st century. Of particular interest is the relationship between economic development (as measured by GDP per capita) and indicators of democratic government. Do democratic reforms inspire economic growth, or are they a symptom of a more prosperous society? This is a question addressed by many studies, which can now be illuminated by systematic analysis of the data collected by the World Bank and the UN, to show how different facets of society are dynamically linked. In this study, published in PLoS ONE (Blomqvist et al. 2018) we improve on methods for identifying these relationships through Bayesian model selection, and we benchmark the models we select against a neural network prediction.

Individual personality predicts leadership in flying pigeon flocks. In a group of flying birds each individual must adjust its direction and speed of motion in line with other group members. Previous studies have shown that some pigeons to make more independent motions, which others then follow in a leadership hierarchy: bird A follows bird B, and bird B follows bird C. These leadership hierarchies are remarkably stable, but how are they determined. Typically the leadership hierarchy in flight is different from that during feeding; the 'pecking order'. In this paper in Phil Trans Roy Soc B (Sasaki et al. 2018) we show that leadership is well predicted by `boldness', a personality trait that represents an individual's willingness to investigate novel stimuli. Bolder pigeons tend to be group leaders, possibly by virtue of being faster to respond to navigational cues, flying faster (and thus getting to the front of the group) or by attending more strongly to novel stimuli and less to other group members.

Surgeons operate more slowly when switching between different types of operation. Surgeons often carry out many operations of different types during the course of a day. We wanted to know whether surgeons became more efficient at performing a given operation throughout the day, as they 'warmed up' on a particular technique. Our analysis showed that performing the same type of operation several times in a row was indeed associated with reduced surgery times, and conversely that switching between different types of operation was associated with longer surgery times. These results, published in the British Journal of Surgery (Pike et al. 2018) [pdf] suggest that surgery scheduling could be optimised to reduce switching, and potentially that practicing a surgery in advance via virtual reality could improve the efficiency of real operations.
Read more: The Times, Daily Mail
Read more: The Times, Daily Mail

Individual personality differences, and environmental differences, explain variation in criminal behaviour. Is crime the result of individuals who are predisposed to break the rules, or do situational factors such as being exposed to rule-breaking opportunities or high-crime environments cause otherwise law-abiding people to commit crimes? In this paper in the European Journal of Criminology (Wikström, Mann & Hardie 2018) we use data from the ground-breaking Peterborough Adolescent and Young Adult Development Study, along with a neural-network based analytical method, to argue that both individual criminogenic propensity and exposure to a criminogenic environment are necessary for a crime to be committed. Our model shows that those individuals with the highest individual propensity are responsible for the majority of all crimes, but that these are overwhelmingly committed in specific crime-inducing environments. This points the way to more targeted crime-reduction interventions.
2017

Optimising collective intelligence with incentives that increase diversity. What do democracy, stock markets, juries and academia have in common? Each depends on the collective wisdom of a group, community or society to make better decisions than a single individual. They can do this because each person brings different information, experiences and perspectives, resulting in a collective intelligence that surpasses any individual alone. But crowds are not always wise. In recent years, prediction markets have failed to predict either Brexit in the UK or President Trump in the USA. In this paper published in PNAS (Mann & Helbing 2017) we show that the way individuals are rewarded for the accuracy of opinions they express has powerful effects on group intelligence. Traditional market rewards (like in a stock or prediction market) tend to encourage everyone to look at the same few data sources. We show that a new reward scheme, designed to reward accurate minority opinions, restores collective wisdom to optimal levels.
Read more: The Conversation, Nature Physics
Read more: The Conversation, Nature Physics

Fish avoid predators by moving more randomly. If a prey fish detects a predator at a distance it has a good chance to outrun it by swimming directly away. At a closer distance the predator may be able to catch the prey if it swims in a straight line, so it makes sense for the prey fish to use turning and speed variations to escape the predator. In particular, the prey needs to move in ways that are unpredictable to the predator in order to avoid interception. In our paper in the Journal of Experimental Biology (Herbert-Read et al. 2017) [pdf] we show that this prediction is borne out in experiments. By measuring the complexity or 'randomness' of the escape path of a fish exposed to a artificial predator, we show that when the predator is closer the prey fish moves more erratically, potentially confusing the predator and giving it a chance to escape.

The importance of maintaining a diversity of approaches in data science. As with many academic fields, data science is subject to waves of fashionable and unfashionable research. In recent years, for example, Deep Learning and complex neural network models have become extremely prominent across most venues publishing research in machine learning. In this article, published in Data Science (Mann & Woolley-Meza 2017), we give our viewpoint on why it is important to maintain a diversity of modelling approaches as an academic community. Using a combination of network science and evolutionary game theory we suggest how we can adapt the structures and incentives that researchers work within to achieve the goal of more diversity and better collective wisdom.

Picking the best collections collectively without communication. Imagine you and your friends go to the shops to buy ingredients for a meal. You need to pick a set of ingredients that will work well together, but you lose each other in the shop and cannot contact each other to coordinate what you should each buy. How can you decide what to get so that the group's meal will be as good as possible? In our paper presented at the Conference on Autonomous Agents and Multiagent Systems (Malkomes et al. 2017) we address problems like this. We investigate algorithms for optimising group collective decisions with communication between agents and apply these algorithms to problems of computational search tasks, automated environmental monitoring and social insect foraging.

Fish make more use of social information when predation is low. Several rivers in Trinidad have a special property that allows the effects of predation to be studied: the lower reaches of the river, nearer the sea, have been colonised by predatory species that originated in the ocean. Higher reaches are protected by the existence of waterfalls that have stopped these species from migrating further upriver. As a result there are spatially-separated populations of guppies (a small prey fish) that have over a long time been exposed to either high or low predation risk. We studied how these different populations used social information when foraging. We found that while guppies from low-predation populations are able to improve their individual decisions about where to forage when in a group, those in high-predation populations were not (or did so sufficiently little as to make any effect statistically non-significant). Our results, published in Behavioral Ecology (Clément et al. 2017) suggest that there is a cognitive trade-off between predator detection and social information use.
2016

Focusing on small data sets to assist hospitals with bed management. There is a lot of excitement about the potential for Big Data to revolutionise the delivery of healthcare, but often a wide gap between the hype and the implementation. In this paper, published in Frontiers in Public Health (Mann et al. 2016) we look at how the unglamorous but essential task of bed management can be assisted by using the data routinely collected at hospitals to predict how long patients will need to stay after an operation. Making use of this existing data could prevent cancellations of elective operations and reduce the number of beds needed to guarantee emergency treatment, saving patients and the NHS time and money.

Problem solving without a brain. All organisms have to solve problems such as how to find food and avoid predators. Some problems can be solved using built-in rules of behaviour, like the way plants can grow towards sunlight. Other problems require learning from past experiences, like remembering your route home from school or work. We learn through changes in the connections in our brains, but what about organisms that have no brain? Can they learn? In this paper in J R Soc Interface (Reid et al. 2016) we show that slime moulds, a simple single-celled organism with no neurons, can solve a classic learning task, the Two-Armed Bandit game. The cell expands more in areas where it has previously encountered food, resulting in an eventual decision to grow only towards higher food sources that mimics simple learning rules for playing the game.
Read More: TreeHugger, Phys.Org, IFLScience
Watch: Simon Garnier describes his lab's slime mould research
Read More: TreeHugger, Phys.Org, IFLScience
Watch: Simon Garnier describes his lab's slime mould research
2015
Visualising and analysing player behaviour in Minecraft. In Minecraft players inhabit a virtual world where they can explore, socialise, fight or collaborate to create new structures and experiences. Using our Heapcraft framework for data analysis and visualisation, we provide tools to help players and server administrators create the worlds they want, and effectively manage their virtual societies. In our paper presented at Foundations of Digital Games (Müller et al 2015) we demonstrated these tools and point the way to using data from Minecraft servers to answer social science questions about how societies are formed and maintained, and what types of social structure enable effective interaction and co-operation. Our upcoming paper at Motion in Games gives full details of all the publicly available tools our project has made available, while our paper at Artificial Intelligence and Interactive Digital Entertainment we show how collaboration between players can be measured and improved using our tools.
|

Causal entropic forces create collective organisation. Following the discovery that systems that maximise their entropy or 'variability' over possible future paths in an uncertain world exhibit intelligent behaviour, we assessed what this type of intelligence implies for collective self-organisation. In our paper in J R Soc Interface (Mann & Garnett 2015) we show that groups of agents who maximise their long-term entropy over possible movements or decisions in the future will naturally interact with each other, using social interaction rules such as Weber's law and 'social forces' that closely match many observed interactions in real animals and humans. This suggests that such groups can be understood via aggregate entropic methods without considering the detailed interactions between group members.
2014

Large groups use negative feedback to avoid overcommitting to one option. Although they are single-celled organisms, slime moulds often act like groups when making decisions. Each part of the slime mould chooses to expand or move based on local cues, creating decision-making behaviour that closely resembles a school of fish or a flock of birds. Collective decisions are often driven by positive feedback - each individual becomes more likely to choose an option if others have already done so. This can often lead to information cascades where the whole group commits to one option, which can lead to other choices being under-exploited. In our paper in PLoS Computational Biology (Zabzina et al. 2014) we show that slime moulds are able to avoid this outcome when they have a large mass (equivalent to a large group), by using negative feedback to prevent too much mass being committed to any single option. Technically this negative feedback creates a new bifurcation for large masses/groups that introduces a new stable state where the slime mould divides itself between options. These results suggest that such mechanisms may regulate group sizes by favouring the splitting of very large groups, and prevent too many individuals trying to exploit the same resources.

Interaction rules, preferred positions, and noise. Previous studies have used a data driven approach to identify interaction rules between individual animals - how each individual changes their movement in response to others. These studies looked for attraction rules - moving towards the neighbour - and alignment rules - moving in the same direction as the neighbour. Our paper in Movement Ecology (Perna et al 2014) shows that attraction towards a preferred position relative to the neighbour can look like an attraction rule or an alignment rule, depending on where the preferred position is. We also show that "noise", either from tracking errors or the animals' intrinsic variable motion can create the illusion of particular interactions even where none exist. Together this means that future studies need to be more careful in how interaction rules are defined and identified.

Inside the mind of a sheepdog. The use of dogs to herd animals is a millenia-old tradition, which is still active today on the farm, the hills and in competitive sheepdog trials - even on television through shows such as One Man and His Dog. That such an established farming practice can make it to our TV screens speaks to the impressive spectacle of watching a solitary dog herding dozens, or even hundreds of sheep, seemingly moving the whole herd at will. How can one animal exert such precise control over so many others? What rules does it use to make sure it moves the herd in the correct direction, and never loses individual sheep? In our paper in J R Soc Interface (Strömbom et al. 2014) we investigate the rules the sheepdog uses, using a combination of agent-based models and data recorded from tracking sheep and dog in real herdings in Australia. Our results show that just two basic rules, driving and collecting, can explain the success of the sheepdog, and in the process naturally explains behaviours such as zigzag motion that previously were assumed to be explicitly learnt by the dog. Photo © AJ Morton
Read more: BBC News, The Times, Independent, Guardian, Daily Telegraph, Daily Mail, Sydney Morning Herald, Speigel Online
Watch: Video comparison of data and model
Read more: BBC News, The Times, Independent, Guardian, Daily Telegraph, Daily Mail, Sydney Morning Herald, Speigel Online
Watch: Video comparison of data and model

Foraging birds choose sites popular with both their own species and other species. In our paper published in Animal Behaviour (Farine et al. 2014) we monitored individual birds trips to feeding stations using radio tags. The birds were more likely to feeders that already had several other birds feeding on them. By fitting a Bayesian decision-making model we assessed how likely individual birds were to pick one of several feeding sites, based on the number of other birds at the site, both conspecifics (birds of the same species) and heterospecifics (other species). While the attraction to an individual of the same species was stronger, the larger number of heterospecifics means that in aggregate the birds of influenced just as much by the presence of other species. Our results show that a simple attraction rule can be a flexible way of finding good food sources in the wild, by exploiting the information provided by other foragers.

The complexity of university education. The performance and experience of university students depends on many factors, which themselves are strongly dependent on each other. Examples include the amount of time devoted to study and the need to work to finance their studies, which both depend on where the student can live and the price of accommodation. In this paper in PLoS ONE (Forsman et al. 2014) we use complexity theory and network analysis to analyse how all these factors influence each other, and how interventions to change any one of the many aspects of student life are likely to change the average performance of students in their examinations.

Mixed personalities are best for foraging groups. The 'personality' of an animal group emerges from the individuals within the group and their interactions. The mixture of individual personalities within the group may lead to overly conservative groups where no-one is willing to take the lead, fractious groups where no-one can agree, or some form of happy medium. In our paper in Proc. Roy. Soc. B (Aplin et al. 2014) we identify different personalities in groups of wild birds, find that birds with different personalities have different social interactions, and then show that a mixture of different personalities is essential to optimise the groups foraging behaviour - no single personality type can effectively explore and exploit the available food sources. These results suggest that we should look to maintain diversity of personality in both animal and human groups.

The dynamics of human development. What causes societies to become more democratic, to increase their people's life expectancy, education and well-being? Is greater education necessary for the development of democracy, or is "people power" required before the government will provide education for the masses? How is a society's administration related to the individual values of its members? Are democracy and permissive values a luxury that only wealthy countries can afford? By applying our previously developed methods to the study of data from the World Bank and the World Values Survey we uncover the dynamical relationships that control changes in individual and societal values and development in this paper published in PLoS ONE (Spaiser et al. 2014)
Watch: Model simulation predictions for India and Ukraine
Watch: Model simulation predictions for India and Ukraine

Hedges and edges help pigeons learn their way around. How do pigeons memorise their way home? By analysing a combination of actual flight paths from homing pigeons, recorded with GPS trackers, and images of the landscape around Oxford where these birds were flying, we try to answer this question in our paper in Biology Letters (Mann et al. 2014). We show that the navigating pigeons were able to return more accurately to areas of the landscape with a special property - these areas contain an optimal amount of 'visual information'. Areas with very little structure, such as open fields, proved difficult to memorise. Likewise very complex landscapes such suburban Oxford were also a problem. But in between lies a 'sweet spot' where the pigeons were best able to recognise and return to locations they had seen before. These areas (shown left), often lying between town and country, or between field and forest, are likely to be key in understanding the foundations of birds visual memories.
Watch: SVT Vetenskapsvärlden, ITV News
Read more: Discover Magazine, Daily Mail, Daily Telegraph, Irish Independent, SVT, Navigation News
Watch: SVT Vetenskapsvärlden, ITV News
Read more: Discover Magazine, Daily Mail, Daily Telegraph, Irish Independent, SVT, Navigation News

Bringing clarity to international development. Our study published in PLoS ONE (Ranganathan et al. 2014) shows how raw data about countries' development, collected by the World Bank and other organisations, can be analysed to discover the relationships that drive economic and social progress. Applying Bayesian methods to automatically select between hundreds of competing models we find the set of differential equations that best explain the development seen in GDP, democracy, child mortality and other measures over the past decades in countries from across the globe. Understanding whether economic growth drives democracy or vice versa, whether education or health is more important for further economic development, and whether there are crucial tipping points in a country's development means we can better predict and assist the path of development in the future. Check out the accompanying R package to apply these methods yourself
Watch: Video abstract
Watch: Video abstract
2013

Fish follow the 'most recent mover' when making decisions. The positions and movements of other individuals provide fish with a valuable source of social information. In particular, the current locations of other fish can indicate where food might be found, and where it is safe to stay. We expect therefore to find fish moving towards larger groups, to take advantage of the safety and information they provide. However, in our experiments, published in J R Soc Interface (Mann et al. 2013), we find that humbug damselfish used different strategy when choosing to move between two coral areas. Instead of trying to find the larger group, each individual simply copied the actions of the last fish it saw moving, leading to cascades of fish moving between the two coral. Such a strategy may help the fish know when it is safe to explore, or to detect when it is best to try new options.
Read more: David Sumpter's Blog
Read more: David Sumpter's Blog

Crowds are wise for hard tasks, but not for easy ones. 'Swarm intelligence' and the 'wisdom of the crowd' are commonly used terms suggesting that groups of individuals can outperform individuals in a variety of tasks. Conversely, the 'madness of crowds' is often invoked to explain disastrous group behaviours, like fatal stampedes and stock market crashes. Our results, published in PNAS (Sasaki et al. 2013), show that ant colonies are much better at solving hard problems than individuals on their own. However, when tasks are relatively simple (such as choosing between a great nest site and a terrible one), individual ants perform better on average than the colony, as the group sometimes blindly follow each other rather than assessing the nest quality themselves. Photo © Takao Sasaki
Read more: Nature, National Geographic, The Atlantic, LA Times
Read more: Nature, National Geographic, The Atlantic, LA Times

Clapping spreads as a social contagion. Our study in J R Soc Interface (Mann et al. 2013) uses Bayesian model selection to show how applause spreads through an audience like a disease, with each individual being 'infected' by the social pressure created by those already clapping. The end of the applause is similarly mediated, with one or two individuals stopping through boredom or tiredness, followed by a wave of other audience members who respond to the increasing pressure to stop. The best model for how clapping starts, spreads and stops suggests that performances of identical quality can generate very different durations of applause.
Read more: Science Magazine, BBC News, The Times,The Guardian, Daily Mail, Slate, Wissenschaft, SvD
Listen: BBC WS Why Factor, BBC R4 Seriously, BBC R4 Material World, NPR, Scientific American, Radio New Zealand
Read more: Science Magazine, BBC News, The Times,The Guardian, Daily Mail, Slate, Wissenschaft, SvD
Listen: BBC WS Why Factor, BBC R4 Seriously, BBC R4 Material World, NPR, Scientific American, Radio New Zealand

Memory of interactions can drive collective motion. Collective motion, such as the swarming patterns of flocks of starlings, or the rotating ball of a school of sardines, is generated by the individual movement choices of the individuals in the group. These choices are driven by the interactions of each individual with the animals around it. Much debate has centred on the question of which neighbouring animals each individual pays attention to - those within a fixed spatial range, a certain number of nearest-neighbours, or only other animals the individual can directly see? In this study in PLoS Computational Biology (Mann et al. 2013) we show that in addition to these spatial limits on interactions, the temporal range must also be considered. Observing the collective motion of glass prawns, we use Bayesian model selection to show that the individual's memory of its recent interactions is important in determining its future direction choices.
2012

Male fish create food-shaped lures to attract females. Male sword-tail charagins have protruding appendages that they use to attract the attention of females, luring them into a position from which they can transfer sperm. It had been suggested that these lures may be attractive to the females because they resemble possible food items, much like a fisherman's 'fly'. In this study published in Current Biology (Kolm et al. 2012) we showed that female fish were more attracted to lures resembling their habitual food sources. After only 10 days on an ant-based diet, females were more attracted to lures that resembled ants, demonstrating both the accuracy of the food-lure hypothesis, and the rapid adaptability of the females' preferences.
Read more: Science Magazine, New York Times, Scientific American, Daily Mail,Huffington Post, Discover Magazine
Listen: Sveriges Radio
Read more: Science Magazine, New York Times, Scientific American, Daily Mail,Huffington Post, Discover Magazine
Listen: Sveriges Radio

How we model collective animal behaviour. In this review, published in Interface Focus (Sumpter et al. 2012) we describe the process of modelling the behaviour of groups of animals, focusing on the interplay between what happens on the individual level and the behaviour of the group as a whole. Based on recent work in various areas of collective animal behaviour, we give examples of how the individual and group level behaviour have been approached, and suggest a cycle of theory-driven and data-driven research to find models that accurately describe how the two are connected.

An optimal strategy for playing Battleships. In this paper, presented at ICML (Garnett et al. 2012), we develop a theory for how to perform optimal binary search. Among many real-world applications for this method, including fraud detection, terrorist identification and resource location, one simple example is the game of Battleships. In this game the players aim to sink each others ships, which are hidden from view, by placing 'shots' into nominated grid spaces. As the players hit or miss with their shots they learn about the possible locations the other player has placed their ships in. By designing a strategy for a sequence of shots which maximise the amount of information the player receives, and thus increases their chances of making as many successful shots as possible, our method can be used to play the game in the most effective way.
Watch: Roman Garnett presents the paper here
Watch: Roman Garnett presents the paper here
2011

Fish adapt their speed and heading in response to their nearest-neighbour. We recorded the movements of shoals of mosquitofish, using video tracking to reconstruct the trajectory of each fish in the group. Using a neural networked based learning algorithm we identified the factors that were responsible for forcing each fish to adjust its speed and direction. Our results, published in PNAS (Herbert-Read et al. 2011) showed that fish slowed down or sped up to stay in contact with the group while avoiding collisions, and turned towards their closest neighbour to maintain group cohesion. Both the speed adjustment and the direction changes were primarily the result of interactions with the closest individual to the focal fish.
Watch: Video of one of our experiments with 4 fish shoaling
Read more: Daily Telegraph , ABC , Discovery, Australian Geographic, My blog posts on this topic
Watch: Video of one of our experiments with 4 fish shoaling
Read more: Daily Telegraph , ABC , Discovery, Australian Geographic, My blog posts on this topic

How to test models of collective motion. There are a lot of different models that are used to describe how animals move together in groups. Because the behaviour of the whole group is an emergent property, different rules describing the interactions between individuals can often produce very similar group movement. This makes it difficult to test how the individual animals are interacting with each other. In this theoretical paper, published in PLoS ONE (Mann 2011), I showed how we can use recorded movements by individual animals to find out which of the current models are the best description of these individual interactions, and what some of the challenges we might face are as we put this into practice.
2010

Information theory reveals how pigeons learn their way home. By repeatedly releasing homing pigeons from the same location and using GPS tracking technology to follow their flights home, it could be seen that these birds memorised a route from the release site to the home loft, which they followed on each flight with increasing accuracy. By using Gaussian processes and information theory we were able to quantify these route memories in terms of the amount of information they contained, and thus establish how quickly the birds learnt their routes in this study published in J R Soc Interface (Mann et al. 2010). By isolating the parts of the route that contained the most information we could also identify which parts of the landscape were being used as landmarks. Photo © Robin Freeman
As part of my PhD thesis I extended these results to show which birds were sharing or receiving information about the route home when flying in pairs.
Read more: My blog posts on this topic, Navigation News
As part of my PhD thesis I extended these results to show which birds were sharing or receiving information about the route home when flying in pairs.
Read more: My blog posts on this topic, Navigation News

Pigeons choose better navigators as their leaders. Studying the flights of pigeons released together in pairs shows that some birds consistently tend to follow others home. By analysing how well each individual had memorised its own way home before being paired with other birds, we found that the birds with the best memories of their own preferred routes tended to be the leaders, while those who were more uncertain about their own routes tended to follow their partner. The results were published in Biology Letters (Freeman et al. 2010)
Read more: The Conversation
Read more: The Conversation

Changes in behaviour of foraging macaroni penguins. Foraging animals typically alternate between periods of active feeding and periods of exploration and travel. Understanding how the animal's conditioning, recent history and life-phase affect the foraging strategy has been restricted by difficulties in accurately identifying the transitions between the different behaviours. In this paper in Marine Biology (Hart et al. 2010) we were able to overcome some of the statistical difficulties inherent in this task, using a Hidden Markov Model approach. Applying this technique to depth recordings of foraging macaroni penguins, we were able to relate the amount of time spent feeding versus exploring to the reproductive cycle stage of the individual penguins
Photo © Jerzy Strzelecki (CC BY 3.0)
Photo © Jerzy Strzelecki (CC BY 3.0)